The Role of Emotionality, Self-efficacy, Rational- and Intuitive- Thinking Styles in Advanced Chess Expertise

Authors

  • Adrienn VARGA Applied Psychology Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Counseling and Psychological Intervention, Master Graduate School, Babeș-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania
  • Eszter Eniko MARSCHALKO Department of Applied Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Babeș-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania, *Corresponding author: eszter.marschalko@ubbcluj.ro https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8008-5371

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24193/subbpsyped.2024.1.07

Keywords:

chess expertise, ELO rating, A-level, emotionality, self-efficacy, rational thinking, intuitive thinking, practice, adults

Abstract

Background: Literature is scarce regarding the psychological predictors of chess expertise and A-level chess performance. Methods: A cross-sectional study was designed and conducted on a total number of 90 Hungarian and Romanian competitive chess players. More than half were males, the average age was 32.07 (SD=12.99). The study aimed to explore the predictive influence of age, gender, number of hours spent practicing, preferences for rational and intuitive thinking styles, self-efficacy, and emotionality on the likelihood of obtaining a publicly accessible ELO rating within the range of 1800-2500 (indicating at least A-level expertise or higher). Binary logistic regression was applied to examine the weight of each predictor. Results: The data evinced the statistically significant role of gender, and rational thinking style on A-level chess expertise and from all the conclusive predictors the most determinant was the rational thinking style which raised the chance of high expertise more than 60 times. Conclusions: Practice contributes positively to the development of A-level competence. However, the most crucial factor in predicting high chess expertise and performance is the preference for rational thinking style.

ZUSAMMENFASSUNG. Hintergrund: Es gibt nur wenig Literatur über die psychologischen Prädiktoren für Schachkenntnisse und A-Level-Schachleistungen. Methoden: Es wurde eine Querschnittsstudie konzipiert und an insgesamt 90 ungarischen und rumänischen Wettkampfschachspielern durchgeführt. Mehr als die Hälfte waren Männer, das Durchschnittsalter betrug 32,07 Jahre (SD=12,99). Ziel der Studie war es, den prädiktiven Einfluss von Alter, Geschlecht, Anzahl der Trainingsstunden, Präferenzen für rationale und intuitive Denkstile, Selbstwirksamkeit und Emotionalität auf die Wahrscheinlichkeit zu untersuchen, eine öffentlich zugängliche ELO-Bewertung im Bereich von 1800-2500 (was mindestens A-Niveau oder höher bedeutet) zu erhalten. Es wurde eine binäre logistische Regression angewandt, um das Gewicht der einzelnen Prädiktoren zu untersuchen. Ergebnisse: Die Daten zeigten die statistisch signifikante Rolle des Geschlechts und des rationalen Denkstils für die Schachkompetenz auf A-Niveau. Von allen schlüssigen Prädiktoren war der rationale Denkstil der bestimmendste, der die Chance auf eine hohe Kompetenz um mehr als das 60fache erhöhte. Schlussfolgerungen: Übung trägt positiv zur Entwicklung der A-Level-Kompetenz bei. Der wichtigste Faktor bei der Vorhersage von hoher Schachkompetenz und Leistung ist jedoch die Präferenz für den rationalen Denkstil.

Schlüsserwörter: Schachexpertise, ELO-Bewertung, A-Level, Emotionalität, Selbstwirksamkeit, rationales Denken, intuitives Denken, Praxis, Erwachsene

References

Allen, M. S., Greenlees, I. & Jones, M. (2011). An investigation of the five-factor model of personality and coping behaviour in sport. Journal of Sports Sciences, 29(8), 841-850. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2011.565064

Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340-345. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890902935878

Betsch, T., & Glöckner, A. (2010). Intuition in judgment and decision making: Extensive thinking without effort. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 279–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2010.517737

Bilalić, M., Smallbone, K., McLeod, P. & Gobet, F. (2008). Why are (the best) women so good at chess? Participation rates and gender differences in intellectual domains. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276, 1161-1165. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1576

Bognár, J. & Orosz, G. (2014). Az ésszerűség-megérzés kérdőív Magyar adaptációja és az ego-rugalmassággal mutatott összefüggései. Pszichológia, 34(2), 129-147. https://doi.org/10.1556/Pszicho.34.2014.2.3

Campitelli, G. & Gobet, F. (2005). The mind's eye in blindfold chess. European Journal of Cognitiive Psychology, 17(1), 23-45. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000349

Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(2), 467–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.2.467

Charness, N., TuYash, M., Krampe, R., Reingold, E., & Vasyukova, E. (2005). The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 151–165. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1106

Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). The Mind’s Eye in Chess. Visual Information Processing, 215–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-170150-5.50011-1

Cumming, J., Hall, C. & Starkes, J. L. (2005). Deliberate Imagery Practice: the reliability of using a retrospective recall methodology. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76(3), 306-314. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2005.10599301

de Groot, A. D. (1978). Section 65: Factors of Chess Talent. In A. D. de Groot (Ed.), Thought and Choice in Chess, 2nd ed. (pp. 356-361). The Hague, The Netherlands, Mouton Publishers.

Dreyfus, H.L., & Dreyfus, S.E. (1988). Mind over Machine: The Power of Human Intuition and Expertise in the Era of the Computer. IEEE Expert, 2, 110-111.

Epstein, S. Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V. & Heier, H. (1996). Individual Differences in Intuitive-Experiential and Analytical-Rational Thinking Styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390-405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390

Epstein, S. (2010). Demistifying Intuition: What It Is, What It Does and How It Does It. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4). https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2010.523875

Ericsson, K. A. & Charness, N. (1994). Expert Performance: Its Structure and Acquisition. American Psychologist, 49(8), 725-747. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.725

Élő, Á. (1978). The Rating of Chessplayers, Past and Present. New York, Arco Pub.

Gigerenzer, G., & Brighton, H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in cognitive science, 1(1), 107–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2008.01006.x

Glöckner, A., & Witteman, C. (2010). Beyond dual-process models: A categorization of processes underlying intuitive judgement and decision making. Thinking & Reasoning, 16(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780903395748

Gobet, F., & Charness, N. (2006). Expertise in chess. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. Feltovich, & R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Gobet, F. & Ereku, M. H. (2016). What is Expertise? Defining expertise is trickier than it seems at first sight. Retrieved from: https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/inside-expertise/201602/what-is-expertise

Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Templates in chess memory: a mechanism for recalling several boards. Cognitive psychology, 31(1), 1–40. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1996.0011

Hackfort, D. & Schinke, R. J. (2020). The Routledge International Encyclopedia of Sport and Exercise Psychology. Volume 1: Theoretical and Methodological Concepts. (1st Ed.). London, Routledge.

Horcajo, J., Santos, D. & Higuero, G. (2022). The effects of self-efficacy on physical and cognitive performance: An analysis of meta-certainty. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 58, 102063, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2021.102063

Howard, R. W. (2011). Longitudinal Effects of Different Types of Practice on the Development of Chess Expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology 26(3), 359–369. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1834

Jianguo, P., Wenying, J., Zhigang, H. & Zhou, N. (2019). The influence of chess training on pupils’ self-efficacy, self-esteem and social anxiety. SHS Web of Conferences, 60, 01002, 1-4. https://doi.org/10.1051/shsconf/20196001002

Kahneman, D. & Frederick, S. (2005). A model of heuristic judgment. In K. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), Thinking and reasoning (pp. 267–293). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Kelly, E. J. (1985). The Personality of Chessplayers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(3), 282-284. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4903_13

Kopp, M., Schwartzer, R. & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Hungarian Adaptation of the General Self-Efficacy Scale. Retrieved from: http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/hungar.htm)

Li, K., Jiang, J., Qiu, L., Yang, X., Huang, X., Lui, S. & Gong, Q. (2015). A multimodal MRI dataset of professional chess players. Scientific Data, 2(150044), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/sdata.2015.44

Moxley, J. H., Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N. & Krampe, R. T. (2012). The role of intuition and deliberative thinking in experts’ superior tactical decision-making. Cognition, 124, 72-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.005

Pallant, J. (2016). SPSS Survival Manual. (6th Ed.). Berkshire, Open University Press.

Raab, M., & Johnson, J. G. (2007). Expertise-based differences in search and option-generation strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13(3), 158–170. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.13.3.158

Ramírez, V. A., Mizrahi, S., & Ruetti, E. (2024). Multilevel analysis of positive emotional induction: Which is the role of gender and valence on cognitive control processes? Physiology & Behavior, 278, 114507. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2024.114507

Reyna, C. & Ortiz, M. V. (2016). Psychometric study of the Rational Experiential Inventory among undergraduate Argentinean students. Revista de Psicología, 34(2), 337-255. http://dx.doi.org/10.18800/psico.201602.005

Saarilouma, P., Karlsson, H., Lyytinen, H., Teräs, M. & Geisler, F. (2004). Visuospatial representations used by chess players: A preliminary study. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(5), 753-766. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000501

Steca, P., Baretta, D., Greco, A., D'Addario, M. & Monzani, D. (2018). Associations between personality, sports participation and athletic success. A comparison of Big Five in sporting and non-sporting adults. Personality and Individual Differencies, 121, 176-183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.040

Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2019). Using Multivariate Statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Villafaina, S., Castro, M. A., Pereira, T., Santos, A. C. & Fuentes-García, J. P. (2021). Neurophysiological and autonomic responses of high and low level chess players during difficult and easy chess endgames – A quantitative EEG and HRV study. Physiology & Behavior, 237, 113454, 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113454

Vollstädt-Klein, S., Grimm, O., Kirsch, P. & Bilalić, M. (2010). Personality of elite male and female chess players and its relation to chess skill. Learning and Individual Differencies, 20(5), 517-521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.04.005

Downloads

Published

2024-06-30

How to Cite

VARGA, A., & MARSCHALKO, E. E. (2024). The Role of Emotionality, Self-efficacy, Rational- and Intuitive- Thinking Styles in Advanced Chess Expertise. Studia Universitatis Babeș-Bolyai Psychologia-Paedagogia, 69(1), 135–150. https://doi.org/10.24193/subbpsyped.2024.1.07

Issue

Section

Articles