FALSE MEMORIES IN ONLINE MISINFORMATION EXPERIMENTAL CONTEXT

Authors

  • Andreea HOROIȚĂ Doctoral School of Applied Cognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Babes-Bolyai University, Republicii Street No. 37, 400015, Cluj-Napoca, Romania, andreeahoroita@gmail.com
  • Adrian OPRE Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Babes-Bolyai University, Republicii Street No. 37, 400015, Cluj-Napoca, Romania, adrianopre@psychology.ro https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4041-0659

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24193/subbpsyped.2022.2.01

Keywords:

misinformation effect; leading questions; suggestibility; false memories; eyewitness testimony; online context.

Abstract

In times of pandemic and afterwards, online platforms and settings have been intensively used. With the purpose of investigating how this setting affected our memory, recent studies have found that memory distortions are present in online environments as well. Therefore, the objective of the present research was to assess misinformation effect in online context, more specifically to assess misinformation effect using leading questions and suggestibility techniques in online format. Our results indicate the presence of misinformation effect through suggestibility, but not through leading questions. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.

References

Berkowitz, S. R., & Loftus, E. F. (2018). Misinformation il the Courtron. In Otgaar, H., & Howe, M. L. (Eds). Finding the truth in the courtroom: Dealing with deception, lies, and memories. (pp. 11-30). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Chandler, C. C. (1991). How memory for an event is influenced by related events: Interference in modified recognition tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(1), 115. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.1.115

Chandler, C. C., & Gargano, G. J. (1995). Item-specific interference caused by cue-dependent forgetting. Memory & cognition, 23(6), 701-708. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200923

Daniel, T. C. (1972). Nature of the effect of verbal labels on recognition memory for form. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 96(1), 152. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033486

David, D., Miclea, M., & Opre, A. (2004). The information‐processing approach to the human mind: Basics and beyond. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60(4), 353-368. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.10250

Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of experimental psychology, 58(1), 17. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046671

Deese, J. (1959). Influence of inter-item associative strength upon immediate free recall. Psychological Reports, 5(3), 305-312. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1959.5.3.305

Del Vicario, M., Bessi, A., Zollo, F., Petroni, F., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., ... & Quattrociocchi, W. (2016). The spreading of misinformation online. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(3), 554-559. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1517441113

Dodd, D. H., & Bradshaw, J. M. (1980). Leading questions and memory: Pragmatic constraints. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(6), 695-704. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90379-5

Doyle, C. M., & Lindquist, K. A. (2018). When a word is worth a thousand pictures: Language shapes perceptual memory for emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 62. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000361

Ecker, U. K., Lewandowsky, S., Cook, J., Schmid, P., Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N., ... & Amazeen, M. A. (2022). The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(1), 13-29.

Geiselman, R. E., Fisher, R. P., Cohen, G., Holland, H., et al. (1986). Eyewitness responses to leading and misleading questions under the cognitive interview. Journal of Police Science & Administration, 14(1), 31–39.

Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 11(3), 501. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501

Greenspan, R. L., & Loftus, E. F. (2021). Pandemics and infodemics: Research on the effects of misinformation on memory. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 3(1), 8-12.

Harris, R. J. (1973). Answering questions containing marked and unmarked adjectives and adverbs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97(3), 399. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034165

Heilman, R. M. (2006). The impact of emotions and individual differences upon economic decision-making: a review of experimental literature. Cognitie, Creier, Comportament/Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 10(3).

Huang, L., & Awh, E. (2018). Chunking in working memory via content-free labels. Scientific reports, 8(1), 1-10. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-18157-5

Huff, M. J., & Umanath, S. (2018). Evaluating suggestibility to additive and contradictory misinformation following explicit error detection in younger and older adults. Journal of experimental psychology: applied, 24(2), 180. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xap000013

Jurchiș, R., Costea, A., Dienes, Z., Miclea, M., & Opre, A. (2018). Evaluative conditioning of artificial grammars: Evidence that non-conscious structures bias affective evaluations of novel stimuli. DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/jkfu5

Jurchiş, R., & Opre, A. (2016). Unconscious learning of cognitive structures with emotional components: Implications for cognitive behavior psychotherapies. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(2), 230-244. DOI https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9743-z

Jurchiș, R., & Opre, A. (2018). A dual process approach to current issues from cognitive behavior therapies: An integrative perspective. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 11(4), 374-409. DOI https://doi.org/10.1007/s41811-018-0023-z

Jurchiş, R. (2018). Fundamental properties of implicit knowledge structures: Implications for Cognitive Behavior Therapies. Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies, 18(2).

Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (2014). Memory attributions: Remembering, knowing, and feeling of knowing. In Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 299-320). Psychology Press.

Lindauer, M. S. (1970). Unambiguity of forms and inequality of labels in studies of effect of language on memory for form. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 30(1), 175–181. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1970.30.1.175

Loftus, E. F. (1974). Reconstructiong memory: Incredible eyewitness. Psychology today, 1974, 116-119.

Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive psychology, 7(4), 560-572. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90023-7

Loftus, E. F., & Loftus, G. R. (1980). On the permanence of stored information in the human brain. American Psychologist, 35(5), 409. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.5.409

Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory, 4(1), 19. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19

Loftus, E. F. (1979). Reactions to blatantly contradictory information. Memory & Cognition, 7(5), 368-374. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196941

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 13(5), 585-589. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3

Loftus, E. F. (1991). Made in memory: Distortions in recollection after misleading information. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 27, pp. 187-215). Academic Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60124-3

Loftus, E. F. (1991). Memory distortions. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 32(4), 645. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084648

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1996). Eyewitness Testimony. Introducing Psychological Research, 305–309. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-24483-6_46

Loftus, E. F., & Zanni, G. (1975). Eyewitness testimony: The influence of the wording of a question. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 5(1), 86-88. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03336715.

McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114(1), 1. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.114.1.1

Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2004). Detection theory: A user's guide. Psychology press.

Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Current opinion in neurobiology, 7(2), 217-227. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80010-4

Nguyen, N. P., Yan, G., Thai, M. T., & Eidenbenz, S. (2012, June). Containment of misinformation spread in online social networks. In Proceedings of the 4th Annual ACM Web Science Conference (pp. 213-222). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/2380718.2380746

Nguyen, N. P., Yan, G., & Thai, M. T. (2013). Analysis of misinformation containment in online social networks. Computer Networks, 57(10), 2133-2146. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comnet.2013.04.002

Opre, A. (2012). Inconstientul Cognitiv. Polirom.

Opre, D., Pintea, S., Opre, A., & Bertea, M. (2018). Measuring Adolescents'subjective Well-Being in Educational Context: Development and Validation of a Multidimensional Instrument. Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies, 18(2), 161.

Pennycook, G., Epstein, Z., Mosleh, M., Arechar, A. A., Eckles, D., & Rand, D. G. (2021). Shifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation online. Nature, 592(7855), 590-595.

Pickrell, J. E., McDonald, D., Bernstein, D. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2016). Misinformation effect. Cognitive illusions: Intriguing phenomena in judgement, thinking and memory, 406-423.

Pahre, R. (1999). Leading questions: How hegemony affects the international political economy. University of Michigan Press.

Reynolds, R. M. (2020). Why Does Misinformation Persist? Cognitive Explanations of the Implicit Message Effect. Michigan State University.

Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803.

Roediger, H. L. (2003). Reconsidering implicit memory. In J,S, Bowers, C,J, Marsolek, Rethinking implicit memory, 3-18. Oxford University Press.

Russo, R., Nichelli, P., Gibertoni, M., & Cornia, C. (1995). Developmental trends in implicit and explicit memory: A picture completion study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59(3), 566-578.

Santa, J. L., & Ranken, H. B. (1972). Effects of verbal coding on recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93(2), 268–278. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032514

Sievwright, O., Philipp, M., Drummond, A., Knapp, K., & Ross, K. (2021). Misinformation effects in an online sample: results of an experimental study with a five day retention interval. PeerJ, 9, e12299.

Schacter, D. L. (2002). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. HMH

Shao, C., Hui, P. M., Wang, L., Jiang, X., Flammini, A., Menczer, F., & Ciampaglia, G. L. (2018). Anatomy of an online misinformation network. PloS one, 13(4), e0196087. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196087

Stoll, E. M. (2021). Misinformation modality and its effects on memory.

Swann, W. B., Giuliano, T., & Wegner, D. M. (1982). Where leading questions can lead: The power of conjecture in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(6), 1025. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.6.1025

Su, Y. (2021). It doesn’t take a village to fall for misinformation: Social media use, discussion heterogeneity preference, worry of the virus, faith in scientists, and COVID-19-related misinformation beliefs. Telematics and Informatics, 58, 101547.

Zaragoza, M. S., Belli, R. F., & Payment, K. E. (2007). Misinformation effects and the suggestibility of eyewitness memory. Do justice and let the sky fall: Elizabeth Loftus and her contributions to science, law, and academic freedom, 35-63.

Weinberg, H.I., Wadsworth, J. & Baron, R.S. (1983). Demand and the impact of leading questions on eyewitness testimony. Memory & Cognition 11, 101–104. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197667

Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 277-295. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145028

Windschitl, P. D. (1996). Memory for faces: Evidence of retrieval-based impairment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 1101. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.5.1101

Visu-Petra, L., Cheie, L., & Benga, O. (2008). Short-term memory performance and metamemory judgments in preschool and early school-age children: A quantitative and qualitative analysis. Cognitie, Creier, Comportament/Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 12(1)

Voinescu, A., & David, D. (2019). The effect of learning in a virtual environment on explicit and implicit memory by applying a process dissociation procedure. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 35(1), 27-37.

Downloads

Published

2022-12-05

How to Cite

HOROIȚĂ, A., & OPRE, A. (2022). FALSE MEMORIES IN ONLINE MISINFORMATION EXPERIMENTAL CONTEXT. Studia Universitatis Babeș-Bolyai Psychologia-Paedagogia, 67(2), 5–23. https://doi.org/10.24193/subbpsyped.2022.2.01

Issue

Section

Articles